Introduction
Reading comprehension
is one of the language skills that Indonesians need to learn at school. This is
because knowing reading has become something important and indispensable for
students due to the success of students who rely primarily on their reading
ability. If there is a lack of comprehension of students reading, there is the
risk of learning failure or at least students would have trouble making
progress (Somadayo, Nurkamto, & Suwandi, 2013).
Reading has major
advantages as reading will extend one's horizons and awareness (MS &
Rachmadtullah, 2018). Reading needs to be introduced before kids join formal
education institutions as early as possible (Wigfield, Gladstone, & Turci,
2016). Students are required to obtain various knowledge, including material,
through reading, and to understand the significance of reading. The reality
that can be seen so far is the students' poor reading skills as there are still
a lot of lazy students to learn or the little interest in reading students
(Hahnel, Goldhammer, Naumann, & Kröhne, 2016). Wendy and Scoths
(1994) clarified that providing them the appropriate resources is a very
effective way to help the students function safely and independently. One of
the methods is vocabulary in the classroom. When we learn a language we need
four language skills to communicate in full. We usually learn first to listen,
then to speak, then to read, and finally to write.
Reading is a dynamic
undertaking and a remarkable accomplishment, as a century of work has shown
(Afflerbach & Cho, in press; Huey, 1908; RAND Reading Study Group, 2002).
Reading has been characterized at different historical times by referring to
particular competencies such as reading the Bible, recognizing direc364 tions,
or answering questions about a text. More recently, strategies have been
employed to describe reading aspects involving intentional control and
deliberate behavioral direction Today, like other teachers and scholars, we use the terms skills
and techniques, both formally and informally, to define aspects of the growth
of children's reading as well as aspects of teacher reading instruction (Paris,
Wasik, & Turner, 1991; Pressley & Afflerbach, 1995). Since at least 50
years, the term reading skills has been used in lecture curricula since
teachers and K–12 students. By comparison, the word strategies became common in
the 1970s to mean the cognitive aspects of the processing of information
Instead of reconciling
the gaps between skills and approaches, they were actually grouped together by
scholars, educators and publishers to be systematic. This phenomenon seems to
be an act of convenience, rather than a method of theory. There are three key
causes of ambiguity in our experiences: numerous colloquial uses, insufficient
interpretations and the incoherent usage of informal documents.
Discussion
The Importance of
Reading Fluency
Reading Fluency
Fluent's Value consists of three main elements: accurate reading of the related
text at a conversational pace with correct prosody or speech (Hudson, Mercer,
and Lane, 2000). After long periods with no practice, a fluent reader can sustain
this output for long periods of time can retain the ability, and can generalize
through texts. A fluent reader is also not easily disturbed and reads in a
flowing and effortless way. The strong correlation between reading fluency and
reading comprehension is the most persuasive reason to focus on teaching
students to become fluent readers (Allington, 1983; Johns, 1993;
Samuels, 1988; Schreiber, 1980). Every feature of fluency has a strong
connection to the comprehension of texts. Without accurate reading of words,
the reader will have no access to the intended meaning of the author, and
incorrect reading of words can lead to misinterpretation of the text. Bad
automaticity in reading words or sluggish, laborious movement through the text
taxes the capacity of the reader to create a continuous understanding of the
text. Poor prosody may lead to confusion through inappropriate or meaningless
word groupings, or through inappropriate expressive applications.
Knowledge and Teaching
Methods
Readers tought to
learn something about linguistics, psychology, sociology, biology, education,
and so on, and be able to explain the target language with adequate precision,
but teachers are also required to learn a range of teaching methodologies,
appropriate study ways, and some clear learning principles. The lecturer must
understand that when dealing with different teaching materials and when meeting
students of varying levels of English proficiency, lecturers must follow
different approaches. To achieve this aim, the lecturer must enrich English
knowledge and develop English skills of the educators through various means
such as listening to English-language programs, watching English TV programs
and regularly browsing the English versions of various news items on the
Internet. Within a term, within order to develop the oral skills of students,
lecturers will find every possible way to become walking encyclopedias so that
lecturers can teach something with ability and ease to every student.
Correlation of Reading
Fluency and Reading Comprehension
The study describes
reading fluency meanings as "... the ability to read text easily,
accurately, and with the proper language." All three dimensions seem
critical of a complete concept of fluency in reading (Dowhower, 1991). The fact
that only through oral reading can be measured two of the three dimensions of
fluency, precision, and expressiveness may have led to the small amount of
attention that fluency has provided until recently. Fluency was seen
primarily as a phenomenon of word recognition and oral reading, and the value
of oral reading pales significantly in contrast with that of silent reading
comprehension. Except, perhaps as early readers in school, lecturers spend the
tiny amount of time doing verbal oral reading relative to silent reading
comprehension. The Literacy Dictionary: On the other hand, the Vocabulary of
Reading and Writing describes fluency as "problems of independence from
word recognition that may impede comprehension" (Harris and Hodges, 1995).
Samuels, a pioneer in
research and reading fluency theory, cites the modification and expansion of
the fluency model to include reading comprehension as a major factor in
elevating the significance of the model in the field of re a ding. He states, "In
order to have a good understanding of reading, the reader must be able to
recognize word s quickly and easily" (Samuels, 2002). A large-scale study
of data from the National Evaluation of Educational Success in Reading clearly
identified the connection between fluency and reading comprehension (Pinnell,
et al., 1995). In that study, 44 percent of participants were found to be
diffluent in reading grade-level materials that they had previously read
silently; the study also showed an important, positive relationship between
fluency in oral reading and success in reading comprehensions. A detailed
description will then seem to link the centrality of fluency to the
comprehension of reading and the construct's defined dimensions. Educators
would propose the following definition: Reading fluency refers to swift,
reliable, accurate word recognition skills that enable a reader to construct
the meaning of the text. Fluency often occurs in correct, quick, verbal oral
reading and is implemented in the process of reading, making it possible to
understand the silent reading. Constructions of Reading Fluency Though
discussion of the construct of reading fluency is found as early as in Edmund
Huey's classic 1908 publication (Chard, Vaughn, & Tyler, 2002), most
fluency discussions trace it to the 1974 seminal article by Le Berge and
Samuels as modern theoretical foundations.
The reader can not
concentrate attention on both processes such that reading can continue quickly
and effectively. Like many beginning readers who have not yet acquired
automatic decoding skills, the non-fluent reader will alternate attention
between the two processes. Constructing meaning which involves putting words
into meaningful units of thought, making inferences, relating information
derived from the text with background knowledge and critically responding to
the meaning. To readers who must alternate between attending to the decoding of
words and the development of meaning, reading is a slow, laborious, wasteful,
counterproductive, and often punishing method. If the limited attention and
cognitive capacity is drained by the processing of decoding words, little or no
capacity is available for the attention-demanding process of constructing and
responding to the meaning of a text. Hence, decoding fluency automaticity is
important for high read achievement rates. Keith Stanovich (1986) also
contributed greatly to elevating the value of reading fluency in a classic
article in which he suggested a reciprocal relationship between fluency and the
amount of reading in which a reader engages. Readers who have gained some
fluency are more likely to indulge in more substantial amounts of reading than
readers who lack fluency. The latter would find it hard and laborious to read.
However, Stanovich goes on to point out that readers develop in all those
skills that lead to fluency and fluency itself as a result of engaging in vast
amounts of reading. Non-fluent readers who avoid re a ding fall further and
further behind.
Fluency has also been
related to theoretical constructs of how reading precedes through developmental
stages. Kuhn and Stahl (2000) summarize how the development of fluency is
linked to the stages of development defined by Chall (1996) and by Ehri (1995).
Chall’s is a broad theoretical formulation that describes several stages of
reading comprehension development besides to decoding; therefore educators will
focus on Ehri’s theory, which focuses on decoding through a stage of fluency
development. In line with the theory of automaticity and the definition of
fluency educators have proposed, Ehri (1998) has noted, “Being able to read
words by sight automatically is the secret to the professional reading of the
text. This helps readers to process words in the text quickly, without
attention drawn to the word itself”. Ehri has developed a carefully researched,
an elegant theory of how readers systematically progress in stages from being
non-readers to the point where they can recognize words effortlessly. Readers
at the Pre-alphabetic Stage of Development have no understanding of the
alphabetic theory that in languages like English, A systematic relationship
exists between the limited number of language sounds (about 40 in the case of
English) and the language's graphic forms, or letters. At this stage children
at-tempt to translate the unfamiliar visual forms of print into familiar oral
language through some visual clue that is part of the print. For example,
children may remember the printed word monkey by associating the descending
shape of the last letter of the word with a monkey's tail. approach and quickly
leads to con-fusion
since my, pony, honey, and many other words would also be read
as monkey based on the selected visual clue. At the Partial
Alphabetic Stage of Development, readers have latched onto the notion that
there is a relationship between the letters and sounds and begin to use that
insight. However, their ability to deal with the complexity of the sounds of
word s results in an incomplete use of that relationship. Therefore, they
prefer to concentrate on the most salient, easiest parts of a word to deal with
and, consequently, use initial and, later, final letters as the keys to a
written word's pronunciation. For example, if readers at this stage of
development are taught that the letter sequence g-o is the word go, they may
focus just on the g and the sound it represents to identify the word. However,
using this strategy of focusing on the first letter, the letter sequences give,
get, gone, and gorilla might also, in-correctly, be identified as go. While
children at this stage of development will make errors in identifying words,
they are in a position to make progress since they have developed the insight
that the letters of a printed word are clues to the sounds of the word. As
children become more familiar with the forms of printed letters, are able to
analyze the sounds that compose words, and become increasingly familiar with
the sounds that letters are likely to represent, they move into the Alphabetic Stage
of Development. Now, even though they may never have seen it in print before,
if they know the sounds commonly associated with the letters b-u-g, they can
think about the sounds for eachof the letters and blend them together to arrive
at the pronunciation of the word bug. Ehri’s theory then indicates that as a
result of encountering the printed word bug several times, as few as four times
according to a widely cited study (Reitsma, 1983), they come to accurately and
instantly identify the word bug without attending to the individual letters,
sounds, or letter-sound associations. Ehri (1998) describes skilled reading in
the following way: “Most of the words are known by sight. The reading of sight
is a rapid acting process. The term sight indicates that sight of the word
activates that word in memory including information about its spelling,
pronunciation, typical role in sentences, and meaning”. This instant, accurate,
and automatic access to all these dimensions of a printed word is the needed
fluency that will allow readers to focus their attention on comprehension
rather than on decoding. It is important to note that the theory and work of
Ehri suggest that this quick, instant recognition contributes to the careful
processing of print in the completely alphabetic stage. Partial alphabetic
readers store in-complete representations of words and, therefore, confuse
similar words such as were, where, wire, wore, etc. However, once the word form
is fully processed, with repeated encounters of the word, it is recognized
instantly. As readers gain skill in processing print, they move into the
Consolidated Alphabetic Stage of Development and also develop another valuable
attention-saving decoding skill.In addition to storing words as units, repeated
encounters with word s allow a reader to store letter patterns across different
words. Using Ehri’s example, the multi letter unit ‘est’ will be stored as a
consolidated unit as a result of reading the words nest, pest, rest, test,
vest, and west. Upon encountering the word chest for the first time, a
consolidated alphabetic reader would need to connect only two units: chand est,
rather than the five units that the fully alphabetic reader would need to
combine. As noted, while this approach to reading a word is faster than
combining the individual phonemes, it is not as quick and accurate as
recognizing the word from the sight.
Modeled Reading
One way of enhancing
fluency is for teachers to read students aloud (Dowhower, 1987; Hoffman, 1987;
Smith, 1979). The student reading aloud process needs to be complemented with
interventions that actually involve students in text interaction, but reading
aloud provides them with a model of how to speed reading in linked text and how
to infuse speech (attend dialog marks and punctuation). Taped or
computer-modelled reading is also a viable way to promote fluency. However,
taped or computer-modeled reading is more effective than no model for younger
and less competent readers but not as effective as a teacher model (Daly and
Martens, 1994). An additional advantage of making text read initially by a
sample increased comprehension for lesser performing readers. It would appear
that the reading model initially required students to concentrate on the
substance of the passage before reading it in-dependently (Monda, 1989).
Although it varies from study to study whether students followed along in
copies of the texts, experts suggest this as a way to include children
individually in the text before reading it.
Conclusion
The findings in this
research are that the Guided Reading Thinking Activity's learning reading
technique has more control over reading, writing, speaking, dreaming, reciting,
and evaluating comprehension skills writing techniques. This is demonstrated by
the average score of students 'reading comprehension skills from high critical
thinking classes using the Guided Reading Learning Activity reading technique
that is higher than the students' average reading comprehension ability score
using Overview, Query, Interpret, Reflect, Recite and Review techniquesDriven
Reading Learning Exercise is the most appropriate reading method used to
develop reading comprehension skills for students. The Guided Reading Learning
Activity reading strategy can be used in groups of students who have high
critical thinking skills. In groups of students with low critical thinking
skills, strategies to read the Guided Reading Learning Exercise and Preview,
Ask, Interpret, Analyze, Repeat and Review can be used. Since the average
reading comprehension score of students who have poor critical thinking skills
using the reading technique Guided Reading Learning Behavior is lower than the
reading preview technique, Question, Read, Reflect, Recite, and Review, it
cannot be proven statistically but significantly.
Afflerbach, P., & Cho, B. (in press).
Identifying and describing constructively responsive comprehension strategies
in new and traditional forms of reading. In S. Israel & G. Duffy (Eds.),
Handbook of reading comprehension research. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Allington, R.L. (1983). Fluency: The neglected
reading goal. The Reading Teacher, 36, 556–561
Dowhower, S.L. (1987). Effects of repeated
reading on second-grade transitional readers’ fluency and comprehension. Reading
Research Quarterly, 22, 389–406.
Hahnel, C., Goldhammer, F., Naumann, J., &
Kröhne, U. (2016). Influencets of linear Reading, basic computer skills,
evaluating online information, and navigation on reading digital text.
Computers in Human Behavior, 55, 486–500.
https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.042.
Hudson, R.F., Mercer, C.D., and Lane, H.B.
(2000). Exploring reading fluency: A paradigmatic overview. Unpublished
manuscript, University of Florida, Gainesville.
MS, Z., & Rachmadtullah, R. (2018).
Constructivism approach in learning to write narrative at elementary school.
1st International Conference on Innovation in Education (ICoIE 2018),
287–290. https://doi.org/10.2991/icoie-18.2019.64.
Paris, S.G., Wasik, B.A., & Turner, J.C.
(1991). The development of strategic readers. In R. Barr, M.L. Kamil, P.
Mosenthal, & P.D. Pearson (Eds.), Handbook of reading research (Vol. 2, pp.
609–640). White Plains, NY: Longman.
Scotts, & windy. (1994). Teaching English
to children. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.
Somadayo, S., Nurkamto, J., & Suwandi, S.
(2013). The influencet of learning model DRTA (Directed Reading Thingking
Activity) toward students’ reading comprehension ability seeing from their
reading interest. Journal of Education and Practice, 4(8), 115– 123
Wigfield, A., Gladstone, J. R., & Turci,
L. (2016). Beyond cognition: Reading motivation and reading comprehension.
Child Development Perspectives, 10(3), 190– 195. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdep.12184.
Komentar
Posting Komentar